Things aren’t always what they seem. This is true, partially because there are many ways to look at something. Seeing an object from one angle may give you a completely different “picture” of the object than if you see it from another angle. Lighting makes a difference. Any objects in your field of vision may block you from getting the full picture of an object.
And yet, the object remains the same. If we are familiar with this object, we can close our eyes and picture the object as it is, even if the proximal stimuli is telling us a different story.
What Is A Proximal Stimulus?
In order to process an object, our sensory receptors have to take information from that object and send it to the brain for processing. The information that our sensory receptors take in is called the proximal stimulus.
Proximal Stimulus Example
For example, we may look at an open book and see the pages that the book is open to. The actual object, the distal stimulus, consists of many more words than just the ones that we see. But the sensory receptors can only measure and process the words on the page, or the proximal stimulus.
You might be saying, but even when I see a closed book, I know that there are words inside. I’ll get to that later.
Proximal vs. Distal Stimulus
We know that the sensory receptors may not be able to take in everything about the object or see it for what it really is. An object in its true form is called the distal stimulus. When we talk about the proximal stimulus, it is often in relation to the distal stimulus.
If “proximal” means immediate or near, “distal” means away or detached. These terms are often contrasted against each other (for example, when talking about “proximal risk factors” or “distal risk factors.”)
This Reddit post dives deeper into the difference between the two and provides references in common textbooks that explore proximal and distal stimuli.
Proximal Stimuli In Different Senses
Each sense uses different receptors to measure and process proximal stimuli. The retina, for example, contains millions of photoreceptors that take in the sight of proximal stimuli.
The Organ of Corti is the sensory receptor for the ear. It is located inside of the cochlea and contains “hair cells” that receive information from the proximal stimulus.
The olfactory receptor cells are responsible for “smelling” the proximal stimulus. These cells are located in the back of the nasal cavity.
Different taste receptors in the taste buds process the sweetness, bitterness, etc. of the proximal stimuli.
Last but not least, touch receptors are located throughout the skin.
Accuracy of Proximal vs. Distal Stimulus In Different Senses
When it comes to some senses, there is very little to differentiate the proximal vs. distal stimulus. Psychologists believe that touch, for example, is pretty straightforward. If we are physically touching an object, we are close enough to get a true sense for what it is.
But things get a little tricky when we talk about vision. Think about seeing an object from far away. It doesn’t always appear to be what it truly is. The orientation of the object makes a big impact on the difference between the proximal stimulus and distal stimulus.
Other characteristics that change the proximal stimulus include touch, balance, light, and position of the object.
Constancy: Our Mind Can “Fill in the Blanks” With Proximal Stimuli
Let’s say you see your cat from a distance. The angle at which its sitting doesn’t reveal the cat’s tail or hind legs. But this doesn’t cause you any concern. You know that those hind legs and tail are still there.
This phenomenon is called perceptual constancy. Your perception of your cat does not change, despite the fact that you cannot see its hind legs or tail. In the past, you have been able to process the idea of what your cat looks like. This additional process is key to “seeing” your cat, even if the proximal stimuli is giving you a different “picture” of your cat.
There are different types of constancy, including shape, lightness, and size constancy.
Invariants
This doesn’t just happen to objects that you are familiar with. Let’s say you see the front of a car approaching you while on a desert road. Without much thought, your mind assumes that there is a back to the car and that it’s an enclosed object.
Or, to pull from another sense, let’s say you hear the sound of a car approaching. Without thinking, you know that there is a car nearby (or on its way.)
During this process, the mind takes in proximal stimuli. The visual system processes that stimuli and recognizes that it’s a car. Your mind then creates a mental representation of the distal stimuli, aka the car.
And that, my friends, is perception.
The mind can easily play tricks on us, but it can also help us out when things don’t appear to be what they seem. As you learn more about perception and psychology, you’ll begin to understand just how fascinating and complicated the mind really is.
[article-reference]