The Spacing Effect in Learning and Retention

practical psychology logo
Published by:
Practical Psychology
on
Courtney Beatey
Reviewed by:
Courtney Beatey, B.Ed

There is one word that college students know better than anyone else. It has nothing to do with fraternities or dorm rooms or beer pong. It’s cramming. Staying up until the early morning hours, cramming as much information as possible into your memory so you can recall it for a big exam the next day. This is the exact opposite of the Spacing Effect.

What Is The Spacing Effect? 

The Spacing Effect is the idea that when you commit to learning information over a long period of time, you are more likely to retain more of that information. A spaced presentation is more effective for learning than a mass presentation.

​Example of the Spacing Effect

Cramming is definitely an example of a massed presentation. You put a large mass of information in front of you and try to memorize it in a short period of time. The Spacing Effect says that if you commit to learning that information by spacing out your study time over a period of weeks, your studying will be more effective.

a woman stressed out over cramming

This graphic on Reddit demonstrates the difference between the Spacing Effect and cramming, suggesting that the former leads to better test scores than a single, intense cramming session.

Many college students claim that cramming works for them. They procrastinate because they believe they can. All their study time is reserved for the night (or even mere hours) before a test. 

But science says that cramming doesn’t exactly work. When you look at the Spacing Effect, it becomes clear that cramming won’t set you up for success, especially if you want to retain the information past the hours when you’re in the exam room. 

As much as college students love to cram information before a test, the Spacing Effect makes a lot of sense. Here are a few reasons why. 

Why Does The Spacing Effect Work? 

The two main reasons that cause the spacing effect seem to be related to how we store memories. Remembering information, after all, is simply storing and retrieving it in long-term memory.

diagram of memory processing, from sensory input to long-term memory

Our Short-Term Memory Storage Is Limited 

While our long-term memory boasts an impressive capacity, allowing us to recall memories from several decades ago, our short-term memory (STM) operates on a more limited scale. Drawing on the findings from Miller (1956) and further clarified by Cowan (2001), humans are typically able to retain about three to five "chunks" of information in STM at any given time. The term "chunks" refers to individual units of information grouped together. Miller initially proposed that this number was around seven chunks; however, Cowan argued that Miller's suggestion was more of a general estimate rather than a strict upper limit.

When faced with information overload, our STM can only accommodate a certain number of chunks before reaching its threshold. This limitation has implications for how we process and store information. If we aim to transfer data from STM to long-term memory, giving it significance or meaning is crucial. This can be achieved by forming an emotional connection or recognizing its practical value. The process of 'chunking' — grouping related items into a single unit — becomes a pivotal strategy in maximizing the efficiency of our short-term memory storage and enhancing the transition of information to long-term memory.

Multiple Presentations Require Recall

When we cram, the information is always fresh in our minds. It’s easy to recall the information when we quiz ourselves because we just learned it. However, moving information from short-term memory to long-term memory requires time. 

A spaced presentation means you must return your focus to the information multiple times. You have to recall the information you previously learned, repeat it (and similar, relevant information), and take a second look at it. This repetition and renewed focus play a significant role in the effectiveness of the Spacing Effect. 

The Information Becomes More Significant 

Studying information multiple times also gives your brain more ways to recall the information. The information you learn in the library may be forgotten in the exam room. But the information you learn in the library, with friends in your dorm, with a tutor in the student center, etc., offers more ways to contextualize the information and more chances for different stimuli to encourage recall. 

Who Invented the Spacing Effect? 

The man behind the Spacing Effect is none other than Hermann Ebbinghaus. This German psychologist is the man behind some of the most fundamental phenomena in learning and memory. 

ebbinghaus forgetting curve

Egginghaus laid the foundational understanding for the spacing effect through his pioneering work on forgetting curves and memorization. His research elucidated that our retention of new information drastically diminishes within the initial 60 minutes but then steadies over subsequent days. For those who cram just an hour before an exam, the stark reality is that this information will likely evaporate quickly from memory. However, Ebbinghaus demonstrated that by repeatedly revisiting and studying the material, the rate of forgetting slows, eventually plateauing, ensuring a more consistent recall of the studied content.

Ebbinghaus's contributions to psychology aren't limited to just the spacing effect. His exploration into the Serial Position Effect and insights into short-term memory have deeply influenced how we understand memory and recall. If you're intrigued by these concepts, delve deeper into my dedicated pages on the Serial Position Effect and short-term memory!

Applying the Spacing Effect to Study Habits

If the Spacing Effect is more effective than cramming, how can one apply it practically to improve study habits?

1. Study Regularly with Breaks: Instead of dedicating 5 consecutive hours in one day, spread that time over a week. Study for an hour, take a short break and switch subjects or topics. This optimizes your ability to retain information and helps reduce burnout.

2. Flashcards and Active Recall: Use flashcards to test your knowledge over extended intervals. Apps like Anki or Quizlet use algorithms based on the spacing effect. When you get a flashcard right, it pushes the card back further in the review cycle, ensuring you review it right around when you're about to forget it.

3. Varied Learning Environments: Study the same material in different environments — the library, a coffee shop, your room. Different settings can provide unique associations, making the information more accessible to recall.

4. Integrate Information: Try interlinking information instead of studying topics in isolation. This form of contextual learning, combined with spaced repetition, can enhance understanding and memory retention.

5. Teach Someone Else: The best way to learn is to teach. After studying, try to teach the content to a friend or yourself out loud. This reinforces the material and helps you identify areas you might be weak.

6. Avoid Multitasking: Focus on one subject at a time while studying. Multitasking can harm the spacing effect as it interrupts the structured study intervals.

Evidence in Modern Times: Technology has made the application of the Spacing Effect even more accessible. Numerous studies have shown that students using spaced repetition software, like Anki or Memrise, outperform peers in tests. These platforms base their algorithms on spacing out card reviews, emphasizing the facts you don't remember as well.

While cramming might offer a short-term solution, the Spacing Effect is the superior method for long-term retention and genuine understanding of the subject. By applying these techniques, students can optimize their study patterns, leading to better performance and less stress.

Reference this article:

Practical Psychology. (2019, August). The Spacing Effect in Learning and Retention. Retrieved from https://practicalpie.com/spacing-effect/.

About The Author

Photo of author